Sumner, Petroc ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0536-0510, Vivian-Griffiths, Solveiga, Boivin, Jacky ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9498-1708, Williams, Andy ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9603-4309, Venetis, Christos A., Davies, Aimee, Ogden, Jack, Whelan, Leanne, Hughes, Bethan, Dalton, Bethan, Boy, Fred and Chambers, Christopher D. ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6058-4114 2014. The association between exaggeration in health related science news and academic press releases: Retrospective observational study. The BMJ 349 , g7015. 10.1136/bmj.g7015 |
Preview |
PDF
- Published Version
Available under License Creative Commons Attribution. Download (2MB) | Preview |
Abstract
Objective To identify the source (press releases or news) of distortions, exaggerations, or changes to the main conclusions drawn from research that could potentially influence a reader’s health related behaviour. Design Retrospective quantitative content analysis. Setting Journal articles, press releases, and related news, with accompanying simulations. Sample Press releases (n=462) on biomedical and health related science issued by 20 leading UK universities in 2011, alongside their associated peer reviewed research papers and news stories (n=668). Main outcome measures Advice to readers to change behaviour, causal statements drawn from correlational research, and inference to humans from animal research that went beyond those in the associated peer reviewed papers. Results 40% (95% confidence interval 33% to 46%) of the press releases contained exaggerated advice, 33% (26% to 40%) contained exaggerated causal claims, and 36% (28% to 46%) contained exaggerated inference to humans from animal research. When press releases contained such exaggeration, 58% (95% confidence interval 48% to 68%), 81% (70% to 93%), and 86% (77% to 95%) of news stories, respectively, contained similar exaggeration, compared with exaggeration rates of 17% (10% to 24%), 18% (9% to 27%), and 10% (0% to 19%) in news when the press releases were not exaggerated. Odds ratios for each category of analysis were 6.5 (95% confidence interval 3.5 to 12), 20 (7.6 to 51), and 56 (15 to 211). At the same time, there was little evidence that exaggeration in press releases increased the uptake of news. Conclusions Exaggeration in news is strongly associated with exaggeration in press releases. Improving the accuracy of academic press releases could represent a key opportunity for reducing misleading health related news.
Item Type: | Article |
---|---|
Date Type: | Publication |
Status: | Published |
Schools: | Psychology Journalism, Media and Culture Neuroscience and Mental Health Research Institute (NMHRI) Medicine Cardiff University Brain Research Imaging Centre (CUBRIC) |
Subjects: | B Philosophy. Psychology. Religion > BF Psychology R Medicine > RA Public aspects of medicine |
Additional Information: | This article has a correction. Please see related URL. This article is Open Access, published under the terms of a Creative Commons licence. |
Publisher: | BMJ Publishing Group |
ISSN: | 0959-8138 |
Funders: | BBSRC, Wellcome Trust |
Related URLs: | |
Date of First Compliant Deposit: | 30 March 2016 |
Date of Acceptance: | 5 November 2014 |
Last Modified: | 06 Jan 2024 04:23 |
URI: | https://orca.cardiff.ac.uk/id/eprint/68310 |
Citation Data
Cited 231 times in Scopus. View in Scopus. Powered By Scopus® Data
Actions (repository staff only)
Edit Item |